LinguiSystems home
Executive Functions Test Elementary
Ages: 7-12   Grades: 2-7

This language-based, standardized assessment will show how students use language to organize their worlds.

Test Set
#34240
$179.95
Add to Cart
Executive Functions Test Elementary Forms (20)
#34241
$41.95
Add to Cart

Test Purpose
Executive functions skills allow humans to internally regulate their behavior to get to a desired outcome.  The Executive Functions Test Elementary is a diagnostic test of language skills that affect executive functions, such as working memory, problem solving, inferring, predicting outcomes, and shifting tasks.  It is designed to assess a subject's ability to use his language skills in various executive functions.

 

Test Description
Executive function capacities begin to develop shortly after birth and continue throughout adolescences and early adulthood (Executive Function: Skills for Life and Learning, The Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University, www.developingchild.harvard.edu/resources/).

The specific tasks of the Executive Functions Test Elementary are constructed to yield information about these areas of executive function and language to assist the professional in identifying areas of strength and weaknesses in a child's language and cognitive communication skills.

 

Subtests

  • Subtest A: Attention and Immediate Memory — Auditory
    This subtest presents short passages with three follow-up questions to determine a student's ability to pay attention to details and remember what she hears.
  • Subtest B: Attention and Immediate Memory — Auditory and Visual 
    This subtest presents illustrations and short vignettes. Students answer questions about what they've heard and seen in the pictures.  There are three questions for each vignette, two require attention to detail and one presents a problem to be solved.
  • Subtest C: Working Memory and Flexible Thinking
    These two executive functions are tested by asking the student to listen to a short passage and answer two "thinking" questions.
  • Subtest D: Shifting
    Being able to shift one's thinking quickly and accurately is an important executive function as it allows one to be adaptive to life's changing demands.  The items in this subtest have two parts; first, the examiner names four items in a category and identifies the category.  The student is then asked to name a member of a similar but different category.

Examiner Qualifications
The test should be administered by a trained professional familiar with language disorders (e.g., speech-language pathologist, psychologist).

 

Test Procedures

  • All items in each subtest are administered in their entirety.
  • Begin with the first item in Subtest A: Attention and Immediate Memory — Auditory.  Read each passage and ask the test item questions in the order listed on the test form.  All items are administered orally.

Testing Time

  • 45-60 minutes

Scoring/Types of Scores
Specific scoring guidelines and criteria for determining correct and incorrect responses for each test item are provided in the Scoring Standards chapter of the Examiner's Manual.  Correct and incorrect responses are also referenced on the test form.  Scoring for each subtest is 1 or 0 for correct/incorrect responses.

  • Raw scores convert to:
    • Age Equivalents
    • Percentile Ranks
    • Standard Scores

Discussion of Performance
This section found in the Examiner's Manual, was developed to guide the examiner to make appropriate and educationally-relevant recommendations for remediation based on a clear understanding of each subtest.

The skills students need to be successful on each subtest are delineated and applied to language and cognitive performance.

Error patterns are identified and the implications for why the student responded incorrectly are given.

General remediation strategies you can incorporate immediately into your therapy program or ask teachers and parents to do are also included.

 

Standardization and Statistics
Several attempts to develop appropriate subtests were discarded as the items did not show strong age progression.  Details of these attempts can be found in the Examiner's Manual.

The final version of the Executive Functions Test Elementary was designed to be administered to children ages seven years of age and older.  Test norms were established on children from 7 years, 0 months through 12 years, 11 months.

Two studies were conducted on the final version: the item pool and standardization studies.

The item pool study consisted of 440 subjects.  The test was standardized on 723 subjects that represented the latest National Census for race, gender, age, and educational placement.  In addition, 476 subjects with language disorders, learning disabilities, and autism spectrum disorders were used in the validity studies.

  • Reliability—established by the use of the following for all subtests and the total test at all age levels:
    • SEM
    • Inter-Rater Reliability
    • Test-Retest
    • Reliability Based on Item Homogeneity (KR20)

The test-retest coefficient is .94 for the total test, the SEM is 4.68 for the total test and the KR20 coefficient is .96.  Given the uniqueness of the test, the clinical population, and scoring criteria, the reliability is considered highly satisfactory.

 

  • Validity—established by the use of construct and contrasted group validity.
    • Contrast Groups (t-values)
    • Point Biserial Correlations
    • Subtest Intercorrelations
    • Correlations Between Subtests and Total Test

Results revealed highly satisfactory levels of item consistency (98%).  Internal consistency estimates are clearly satisfactory.  The test differentiates students with language disorders, autism spectrum disorders, and learning disabilities from students with typical language development.

 

  • Race/Socioeconomic Group Difference Analyses—conducted at the item and subtest levels.  The analysis of performance differences among race/socioeconomic groups was conducted at the subtest level.
    • Z-tests Chi Square analysis at the subtest level
    • Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) F-tests

In 67 of the 72 statistical tests for race and socioeconomic combinations, there were no significant racial or socioeconomic differences.  This low occurance for race or socioeconomical differences on test performance indicate that neither is a strong impact on the Executive Functions Test Elementary at the item level.

 

Copyright © 2014

Components
Test Set includes: examiner's manual, picture stimuli book, 20 test forms
  • Children with specific language impairment (SLI) may have deficits in attention, narrative language skills, and auditory working memory (Hoffman & Gillam, 2004; Marton & Schwartz, 2003).  Thus, children with SLI may be at a particular risk of academic failure if they do not learn skills to compensate for weak executive functions.
  • Executive function intervention (including specific tasks to improve student attention, problem solving, and organizational tasks) is vital for children with language-learning disorders (including children with autism) as these skills are needed for academic and social success (Singer & Bashir, 1999).
  • Working memory is one of several factors that can influence the development of children's comprehension skills (Cain, Oakhill, & Bryant, 2004).
  • Targeted educational intervention may help decrease the impact of executive function deficits in children with autism spectrum disorders (Happé, Booth, Charlton, & Hughes, 2006).  The most effective executive function intervention tasks will easily assist the student in improving his academic strategies across a variety of subject areas.  Tasks that show the student how improvement in attention and working memory will help him across a variety of home and school tasks will be intrinsically motivating to the student (Singer & Bashir, 1999).

Executive Functions Test Elementary incorporates these principles and is also based on expert professional practice.

References

Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31-42.

Happé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Examining profi les across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 25-39.

Hoffman, L.M., & Gillam, R.B. (2004). Verbal and spatial information processing constraints in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 114-125.

Marton, K., & Schwartz, R.G. (2003). Working memory capacity and language processes in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1138-1153.

Singer, B.D., & Bashir, A.S. (1999). What are executive functions and self-regulation and what do they have to do with language-learning disorders? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 265-273.

Author(s)

Linda Bowers, Rosemary Huisingh

Biography

Linda Bowers, M.A., SLP, is a co-founder and co-owner of LinguiSystems.  She is a speech-language pathologist with wide experience serving language-disordered students of all ages.  Linda has keen professional interest in the thinking, listening, and language abilities of children and adults.

Rosemary Huisingh, M.A., SLP, is also a co-founder and co-owner of LinguiSystems.  As a speech-language pathologist, she has successfully served the communication needs of school children for many years.  Rosemary is particularly interested in childhood language, vocabulary, and thinking skills.